Conventional wisdom believes “screen time” disrupts mental development, but research hints at a more complicated relationship between our minds and digital technology.
We’ve been here before. When books were the fresh new tech, Socrates believed they would spread an epidemic of forgetfulness. A millennium later, aristocrats fretted that the printing press would lead to mental overload among the masses. Then parents worried that calculators handicapped arithmetic skills and that e-mail would prove more harmful to IQ than pot.
Now, there’s a new mind-mushing invention on the scene: digital technology.
According to a PBS poll, 53 percent of people believe that technology is making us dumber. Polling more than a thousand experts, the Elon University’s Imagining the Internet Center and the Pew Internet Project found that 42 percent believed “the hyperconnected brain is shallow” and maintains “an unhealthy dependence on the Internet and mobile devices.” And Nicholas Carr’s Pulitzer Prize finalist book, The Shallows: How the Internet Is Changing the Way We Think, Read and Remember, says it right in the title.
But the worry over digital technology’s place in the classroom isn’t just the latest flare up of mob technophobia. It’s fueled by high-profile events coinciding with the mass adoption of digital tech among students, leading to a strong associative relationship.
Digital technology enters the classroom
Consider Finland. At the beginning of the century, Finland’s education system gained renown as the best in the world. It was a top performer in the 2000 Program for International Student Assessment (PISA), scoring high in math and science and number one in reading. Educators flocked to the country to uncover its secret pedagogic spice.
But between 2006 and 2012, the country’s scores fell sharply while other top performers remained steady. Several theories have been proffered for the trend reversal, among them the increased adoption of “screen time” technology.
As educator and policy adviser Pasi Shalberg told the Washington Post, Finnish girls outperform boys in reading, mathematics, and science. Finland is the only OECD country where girls outperform boys in the latter two subjects
Girls generally read for pleasure more than boys, and PISA test questions lean heavily on reading comprehension. As such, the appearance of digital technologies among school-aged children may have “accelerated this trend” — with boys’ diminishing reading skills anchoring their test scores down.
Shalberg further posits that increased time spent on the internet for media and socializing may lead to difficulties in concentrating on complex issues, such as those found in math and science.
Another high-profile example comes from the United States, where technology’s introduction into the classroom has been met with mixed results. As reported by the New York Times, Kansas students have staged sit-ins and walkouts to protest the use of Summit Learning Platform. Meanwhile, a Connecticut school district has suspended use of the same digital education system.
A personalized learning system backed by Mark Zuckerberg and Priscilla Chan, Summit Learning uses online tools to generate customized education aimed at promoting self-directed learning. However, some students have found the screen-focused lessons isolating and anxiety-inducing, while parents worry over the effects an untested system will have on their children’s mental development.
We the Parents, a parental organization that opposes mass-customized learning, believes systems like Summit are risky given their lack of proven efficacy. In a letter to the Indiana Area School District board, one member spelled out their concerns over Summit, including the argument that screen-based education removes children from the interpersonal connections that facilitate proper learning.
The letter states: “But lack of evidence does not give us ‘a pass’ to proceed without caution, and the truth is we have many clues that do not bode well when it comes to heavy uses of technology and our children’s educational or socioemotional wellbeing” and “there is no real way to assess his learning outcomes until this little experiment on our children is measured later, after the damage has been done.”
In other words, we are social learners, not digital ones.
Can we determine digital technology’s lingering effects?
Examples like these have primed popular imagination to distrust digital technology’s role in our cognitive development and maintaining mental acuity. But some recent studies have complicated the issue.
“There have been so many books and articles about how we may be relying so much on technology that we are losing some of our cognitive abilities … but it hasn’t been well studied. I can count on one hand the number of people studying the lingering effects of smartphone usage,” Peter Frost, a professor of psychology at Southern New Hampshire University, told the Concord Monitor Report.
Deciding to analyze those lingering effects, Frost took his question and performed a study. First, Frost and his team analyzed college student phone usage and short-term cognitive abilities. They found that more smartphone usage correlated negatively with social problem-solving, but positively with the ability to make observations and judge the credibility of information.
He then assigned 50 undergrads to use their phones for less than two hours a day, while another group of 50 was assigned to more than five hours a day. At the one-week mark, the high-use students showed a diminished ability to interpret and analyze data. But at the four-week mark, that difference disappeared.
“The findings of this study suggest that, even in the rare cases where smartphones might alter cognition, this effect is likely transitory [and that] the mechanism by which smartphones initiate this temporary change remains an open question,” Frost writes.
Another study, reported on in New Scientist, found that children who interacted with screens developed fine motor skills earlier, and no correlation was found that screen time interfered with developmental milestones like learning to walk and talk.
“[Digital technologies offer] unprecedented power, but there are still many important questions about these maddening, valuable devices that we have been unable to answer. What is clear, however, is that many initial reactions have been more knee-jerk than evidence-based,” writes New Scientist consultant Douglas Heaven.
But you may have noticed something missing: causal links.
While the adoption of digital technology predates Finland’s score drops, there’s no direct evidence to suggest cause and effect. Another possible explanation offered by Shalberg includes Finland’s post-2008 economic hardships. And although Summit Learning touts a collaboration with Harvard researchers, it has not let researchers study its specific platform.
Looking to the studies, we stumble into a chicken-and-egg problem. Do the students with improved judgement bolster such skills with their phones, or are students with such abilities more prone to high-usage? Does the phone help toddlers practice fine motor skills, or do more advanced children simply reach for the digital technology sooner?
Learning in the face of uncertainty
In many ways, researchers studying digital technology’s effects on students face the same barriers as the nutritionist. Whether looking at diets that are digital or nutritive, it’s difficult to persuade people to change their lives substantially over a protracted period of time. How many people do you know that would freely renounce all digital technology in the name of science? Or parents that would assign their child to a digital regimen where the deleterious effects are unknown?
And even should people agree, they can’t be put in a lab for years to prove they stuck to the program. Our digital-laced reality means variables will creep into the data, and researchers end up relying on surveys to gather results.
None of this is to say that science can’t ultimately provide evidence-based answers; just that such evidence is tricky to suss out and that digital technology is new and changing rapidly.
In the face of such uncertainty, many experts argue we should avoid the indiscriminate adoption of digital technology. Instead, our approach should be one of intention, only adopting the technologies we need to achieve a desired outcome.
This is the philosophy espoused by Cal Newport in his book Digital Minimalism, Douglas Rushkoff’s Team Human podcast, and websites like the Tech Edvocate. Some developers are also adopting this philosophy, such as the digital-learning platform Cerego.
Cerego’s adaptive-learning tools are designed to nurture learning and long-term retention. Students engage with the platform for cognitive work, but the lessons are spaced out to give their minds time to consolidate the information and to allow for non-digital learning experiences. The goal is to build stronger neural connections with the information, and approach it from multiple angles.
This approach stands in contrast to other digital systems, which profit through each point of engagement and so distract with continuous notifications designed to keep you on the platform.
“If I offered you an ax, you could use it as a tool of incredible destruction, or it could be a great benefit to you,” Lewis said in an interview. “It’s all about finding the right tool for the right mission. But remember: you wield the ax, not anybody else.”
In a case study with Arizona State University’s Global Freshman Academy, astronomy and health-and-wellness students who used Cerego and completed all the course sets scored better than students who did not, suggesting improved retention of foundational knowledge. (Though, in keeping with our theme, these results are correlative.)
And we’ve been here before. When calculators became widespread in elementary schools, parents and pundits worried that they would irrevocably harm the students’ ability to learn mathematics. But math teachers chose to integrate them into the classroom with intentionality. Today, they teach students the “selective and strategic use” of calculators, improving not only math skills but reasoning and problem-solving skills in general.
As the evidence on digital technology continues to be cataloged, it seems the best approach is to consider it neither salubrious nor harmful. As such, the question shouldn’t be whether they make students stupid. It’s whether we are employing them in a way that deters or promotes mentally engaging activities.
- Published Date: 07 October, 2019
- Source: Bigthink.com
naturally like your web site however you need to take a look at the spelling on several of your posts. A number of them are rife with spelling problems and I find it very bothersome to tell the truth on the other hand I will surely come again again.
I cannot thank you enough for the wealth of knowledge and resources you’ve provided on this website. Your commitment to education is truly inspiring, and I’m incredibly thankful. Thank you for making learning so accessible and enjoyable!. It’s a commendable effort, and I’m grateful for the insights I’ve gained. Thanks! ID : CMT-WC4KYO5RRHD7WIFI5T
Thank you. Happy to hear our work is making you happy! -Ki